Language and Mental Health
Lead Research Organisation:
Durham University
Department Name: Philosophy
Abstract
The relationship between language and thought has preoccupied philosophers since antiquity. Modern philosophising on the subject begins with Descartes and the rationalists. They regarded thought as universal, immutable, and rational, whereas language was merely expressive of it. Language was seen as an overt manifestation of thought, and could vary even when thought itself is not. Modern generative linguistics, by and large, adopts this point of view as well, regarding language as a cognitive 'module' that is relatively independent of thought. Within these views, language is not regarded as an organising principle for thought. Thought is taken to be organised by its own principles. In line with this common conceptualisation, language is not regarded as relevant for what it is to be rational, or for mental health. Language could fall away, and rationality and selfhood could be maintained. An alternative and older tradition in linguistics, however, opposes the separation of language and thought. Identifying a mode of thought that is only found in humans, this tradition maintains that language does not just express thoughts, but generates them. Without the generative mechanisms of language, and of grammar in particular, this particular mode of thought wouldn't exist. We call this an 'Un-Cartesian' view of grammar. The present project starts from a prediction that the Un-Cartesian view makes: where thought gets disorganised, grammar should get disorganised alongside, and vice versa. The Un-Cartesian view regards a particular set of grammatical principles as crucial to the organisation of meaning in language. Therefore, it makes specific prediction for which aspects of thought should break down when the grammatical principles in question do. We will explore these predictions through investigations of language in schizophrenia. Formal Thought Disorder (FTD) is one clinical symptom of schizophrenia, and which is diagnosed on the basis of abnormalities in language. In particular, we will examine if grammar as opposed to lexicon (vocabulary) is disrupted in people with FTD.
Apparent evidence against the Un-Cartesian view comes from acquired language disorders, for example conditions such as agrammatic aphasia following a stroke. In this case, language can fail to be understood and produced, yet the thoughts of the people concerned can seem normal. In the case of aphasia, there is initial evidence that the pattern of language breakdown is very different from language abnormalities found in schizophrenia. In this project, we will explore the behaviour of people with aphasia on the same tasks as those with schizophrenia in order to discover whether their grammatical profiles are distinct.
The present project assembles a multidisciplinary research team that crosses the boundaries of departments, faculties, and disciplines. We carry out theoretically guided research on language in the clinical context, and modify those theories in response to clinical evidence. We aim, in a first subproject, to extend our current research on the grammatical basis of reference in language (what speech is about). We then aim to sample spontaneous speech from people with schizophrenic thought disorder and to test whether these grammatical behaviours are specifically impaired. We also use a battery of newly designed language tests to profile language abnormalities in aphasia and schizophrenia. Finally, we will accompany our experimental work with a philosophical study assessing and rethinking the role of language for being a rational self. We hope that our project will contribute new tools for language assessment in clinical populations and generate fresh insights into both aphasia and schizophrenia, as well as shedding light on complex debates on the relationship between language and thought.
Apparent evidence against the Un-Cartesian view comes from acquired language disorders, for example conditions such as agrammatic aphasia following a stroke. In this case, language can fail to be understood and produced, yet the thoughts of the people concerned can seem normal. In the case of aphasia, there is initial evidence that the pattern of language breakdown is very different from language abnormalities found in schizophrenia. In this project, we will explore the behaviour of people with aphasia on the same tasks as those with schizophrenia in order to discover whether their grammatical profiles are distinct.
The present project assembles a multidisciplinary research team that crosses the boundaries of departments, faculties, and disciplines. We carry out theoretically guided research on language in the clinical context, and modify those theories in response to clinical evidence. We aim, in a first subproject, to extend our current research on the grammatical basis of reference in language (what speech is about). We then aim to sample spontaneous speech from people with schizophrenic thought disorder and to test whether these grammatical behaviours are specifically impaired. We also use a battery of newly designed language tests to profile language abnormalities in aphasia and schizophrenia. Finally, we will accompany our experimental work with a philosophical study assessing and rethinking the role of language for being a rational self. We hope that our project will contribute new tools for language assessment in clinical populations and generate fresh insights into both aphasia and schizophrenia, as well as shedding light on complex debates on the relationship between language and thought.
Planned Impact
The research we propose is primarily addressed to three kinds of community: the scientific community across a range of disciplines including philosophy, linguistics, psychology, psychiatry; clinicians in psychiatry, clinical psychology and speech and language therapy; and people with schizophrenia, people with aphasia and their families. In regard to Impact, our central interest is to contribute to a new understanding of the role and importance of language in schizophrenia and mental health more generally. Based on that, there may be potential for development of new psychological and behavioural therapies for the condition. In particular, our research could contribute to improved engagement techniques in Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for people with schizophrenia and new techniques in this therapy for the treatment of thought disorder and negative symptoms such as alogia.
In addition to increasing understanding of language impairments in schizophrenia and aphasia, the project is likely to produce new methods for evaluation of language in these conditions. The implicit language processing tools that will be developed and refined across the course of the project are not routinely used in clinical evaluation of language impairments. One component of subproject B will be to produce computerised assessment methods suitable for adoption in a wide range of clinical contexts. Furthermore, these implicit language assessments will be normed on healthy age-matched control samples in order to increase their clinical utility. These new tools may be refined and disseminated for use by NHS clinicians, in particular, speech and language therapists and neuropsychologists.
Methods and outcomes of research will be reported not only in standard academic outlets, but also in publications and conferences that are clinician-led. For example, at the American Psychiatric Association Annual meeting, ECNP and at the bi-annual schizophrenia research forum meeting, and in speech and language therapy in the annual conference of the Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists and biennial conference of the British Aphasiology Society. We will also engage with local special interest groups (e.g., Speech & Language Therapy in Mental Health) in disseminating our ideas and findings and seek feedback on the project in order to stimulate future development and refinement.
During the course of the project, we will engage with charities such as Mind and Rethink and NAMI. We will invite a member of service user group to join our project steering committee in order to benefit from their insights and reflections on our plans, and also to find ways to share the findings of the project with people with schizophrenia and their families. Similarly, we would engage with AphasiaNow (http://www.aphasianow.org/) an organisation led by people with aphasia and explore ways in which our research findings can be disseminated to people with language impairment.
The relationship between language, mind and thought is one which is of considerable interest to the media, particularly where it is illuminated by evidence from people with neurological or psychiatric difficulties. For example, previous work on aphasia and thought was reported on BBC News website, The Guardian, The Times and New Scientist. Professor Varley has given interviews to the BBC Radio 4 programme Word of Mouth (on inner speech and thought) and on Richard and Judy (language problems following stroke). Professor Turkington has presented on Radio America on the subject of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for psychotic symptoms, including thought disorder.
In addition to increasing understanding of language impairments in schizophrenia and aphasia, the project is likely to produce new methods for evaluation of language in these conditions. The implicit language processing tools that will be developed and refined across the course of the project are not routinely used in clinical evaluation of language impairments. One component of subproject B will be to produce computerised assessment methods suitable for adoption in a wide range of clinical contexts. Furthermore, these implicit language assessments will be normed on healthy age-matched control samples in order to increase their clinical utility. These new tools may be refined and disseminated for use by NHS clinicians, in particular, speech and language therapists and neuropsychologists.
Methods and outcomes of research will be reported not only in standard academic outlets, but also in publications and conferences that are clinician-led. For example, at the American Psychiatric Association Annual meeting, ECNP and at the bi-annual schizophrenia research forum meeting, and in speech and language therapy in the annual conference of the Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists and biennial conference of the British Aphasiology Society. We will also engage with local special interest groups (e.g., Speech & Language Therapy in Mental Health) in disseminating our ideas and findings and seek feedback on the project in order to stimulate future development and refinement.
During the course of the project, we will engage with charities such as Mind and Rethink and NAMI. We will invite a member of service user group to join our project steering committee in order to benefit from their insights and reflections on our plans, and also to find ways to share the findings of the project with people with schizophrenia and their families. Similarly, we would engage with AphasiaNow (http://www.aphasianow.org/) an organisation led by people with aphasia and explore ways in which our research findings can be disseminated to people with language impairment.
The relationship between language, mind and thought is one which is of considerable interest to the media, particularly where it is illuminated by evidence from people with neurological or psychiatric difficulties. For example, previous work on aphasia and thought was reported on BBC News website, The Guardian, The Times and New Scientist. Professor Varley has given interviews to the BBC Radio 4 programme Word of Mouth (on inner speech and thought) and on Richard and Judy (language problems following stroke). Professor Turkington has presented on Radio America on the subject of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for psychotic symptoms, including thought disorder.
Organisations
Publications
A Tovar
(2018)
The linguistic signature of hallucinated voice talk in schizophrenia.
in Schizophrenia Research
Deamer F
(2015)
The speaker behind the voice: therapeutic practice from the perspective of pragmatic theory
in Frontiers in Psychology
Deamer F
(2019)
Non-literal understanding and psychosis: Metaphor comprehension in individuals with a diagnosis of schizophrenia.
in Schizophrenia research. Cognition
Deamer, F.
Metaphorical Thinking and Delusions in Psychosis
in Philosophical Psychology
Fuentes-Claramonte P
(2019)
The interfering effects of frequent auditory verbal hallucinations on shadowing performance in schizophrenia
in Schizophrenia Research
G Sevilla
(2018)
Deficits in nominal reference identify thought disordered speech in a narrative production task.
in Plos One
Hardy CJ
(2016)
The Language Profile of Behavioral Variant Frontotemporal Dementia.
in Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD
Hinzen W
(2015)
The image of mind in the language of children with autism.
in Frontiers in psychology
Hinzen W
(2017)
Reference Across Pathologies: A New Linguistic Lens on Disorders of Thought
in Theoretical Linguistics
Hinzen W
(2014)
The future of universal grammar research (Editorial introduction)
in Language Sciences
Description | 1. Linguistic markers of FTD: Formal thought disorder (FTD) is clinically manifest as disorganized speech, but its is not clear what identifies it in linguistic terms. Here we make a case that the referential function of language, as expressed in specific grammatical patterns, is inherently related to the disturbance of thought seen in FTD. Language samples from thirty participants with schizophrenia (SZ), fifteen with FTD (SZ+FTD) and fifteen without (SZ-FTD), as well as fifteen first-degree relatives of people with schizophrenia (FDRs) and fifteen neurotypical controls (NC) were analysed. We predicted that anomalies in the normal referential use of noun phrases (NPs), sub-divided into definite and indefinite NP types, would identify FTD; and that FTD would also be linked to reduced linguistic complexity as specifically measured by the number of embedded clauses or of grammatical dependents. Results: Participants with SZ+FTD made more referential errors than all other groups and produced the fewest definite NPs, while FDRs produced the most. When referential errors were classed according to the NP type in which they occurred, errors in definite NPs distinguished groups, with the SZ+FTD group making the most definite NP errors. While syntactic errors did not distinguish groups, the SZ+FTD group exhibited significantly less syntactic complexity than non-SZ groups. Exploratory regression analyses suggested that production of definite NPs also distinguished the two SZ groups. These results show that FTD maps onto a linguistic profile and can be directly identified in specific grammatical terms. 2. Sentence comprehension in SZ and FTD: It is still unclear which linguistic impairments affect people with schizophrenia (SZ), how specific these are to formal thought disorder (FTD), to what degree they affect comprehension, and what their neurocognitive basis is. Methods: We here tested for the first time a specific aspect of syntactic complexity in comprehension: the embedding of a clause (e.g., it is cold outside) as an argument of a verb such as think, as in The man thinks it is cold outside. Such sentences all express mentalizing but can be either 'factive' (The man knows it is cold outside) or 'non-factive' (The man thinks it is cold outside). Performance on a sentence-picture matching (SPM) task was compared across these two conditions and four groups: SZ with FTD (SZ+FTD), SZ without FTD (SZ-FTD), first degree relatives of people with SZ (FDR), and neurotypical controls (NC). Results: Total accuracy scores in the SPM task distinguished the SZ+FTD group from all others, with SZ+FTD performing poorly in both the factive and non-factive conditions. Within the SZ+FTD group, participants performed significantly worse in the non-factive condition than the factive one. Accuracy scores in the combined SZ group correlated with standardized sentence comprehension scores (TROG-2), visual semantic memory (PPT), executive functioning (Brixton), and general IQ (WASI-2). These results demonstrate language dysfunction specific to FTD in a critical aspect of syntactic complexity in comprehension, particularly in non-factive clauses, indicating a linguistic dimension within a more general cognitive dysfunction in FTD. 3. Non-literal understanding and psychosis: Metaphor comprehension in schizophrenia: Previous studies suggest that understanding non-literal expressions, and in particular metaphors, can be impaired in people with schizophrenia, although it is not clear why. We explored metaphor comprehension capacity using a novel picture selection paradigm; we compared task performance between people with schizophrenia and healthy comparator subjects and we further examined the relationships between the ability to interpret figurative expressions non-literally and performance on a number of other cognitive tasks. Eye-tracking was used to examine task strategy. We showed that even when IQ, years of education, and capacities for theory of mind and associative learning are factored in as covariates, patients are significantly more likely to interpret metaphorical expressions literally, despite eye-tracking findings suggesting that patients are following the same interpretation strategy as healthy controls. Inhibitory control deficits are likely to be one of multiple factors contributing to the poorer performance of our schizophrenia group on the metaphor trials of the picture selection task. 4.Verbal and nonverbal cognition in SZ, FTD, and aphasia: Patients with schizophrenia show widespread impairments in cognitive performance, including language. Language abnormalities have been considered to be the expression of thought disorder and to be consequent on the broader cognitive impairment, but may also play a causal role in disorders of the form and content of thought. We recruited a clinical population of n=50 (n=30 with schizophrenia and n=20 with aphasia) and n=61 non-clinical comparisons (n=45 healthy controls and n=16 non-affected first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia), in order to establish cognitive and linguistic profiles of these groups and compare them in terms of composite verbal and nonverbal cognitive scores. Results showed linguistic deficits in both clinical groups. These were most severe in participants with aphasia but did not correlate with non-linguistic cognitive performance there, unlike in schizophrenia, where nonverbal cognitive performance was also more affected. Formal thought disorder was associated with worse performance in executive function and in grammatical, but not simple naming, tasks. The data suggest that language impairment as measured on standardized tests can be a different phenomenon depending on the population, being an expression of a broader cognitive breakdown in schizophrenia but not in aphasia, where greater dissociation is seen. These results underline the need for unveiling the qualitative nature of language impairment in schizophrenia so as to understand the role of language in the neurocognition of this disorder. 5. Factive and counterfactive interpretations in aphasia and their relationship with lexical, syntactic and conceptual capacities: In factive clausal embedding ([He knows [that it is warm outside]]), the embedded clause is presupposed to be true. In non-factive embedding ([He thinks [that it is warm outside]]) there is no presupposition, and in counterfactive embedding ([It only seems [that it is warm outside]]) the embedded clause is presupposed to be false. These constructions have been investigated as a window into the complexity of language and thought, and there are disputes as to the relative contributions of lexical, syntactic or non-verbal resources in their interpretation. We designed a sentence-picture matching task to test comprehension of these constructions in a group of aphasic participants and in neurotypical controls. In particular, we tested the capacity to reach a factive or counterfactive interpretation. In factive interpretation trials, participants with aphasia performed nearly as well as controls, while in counterfactive interpretation trials they performed significantly worse. Accuracy in factive and counterfactive interpretation trials correlated with syntactic and lexical measures. Only performances on counterfactive trials correlated with non-verbal reasoning measures. Exploratory regression models suggest that both are separate factors. Results indicate associate that a disruption of counterfactive interpretation in aphasia is linked to reduction of syntactic and/or conceptual-propositional capacities. 6. We investigated whether thought pathology would show on the more 'surface' side of language production as well, namely in disfluency patterns. We annotated the same spontaneous speech sample used in Cokal et al. for empty and filled pauses, as well indexing all pauses by duration and syntactic position. Results of the paper (under revision, PlosOne) show that (i) thought pathology is visible in pausing patterns, and the latter differ in patient with and without FTD; (ii) differences between patient groups and control only appear when looking pauses together with their syntactic positions; (iii) the clausal boundary transpires as particularly prominent, in direct contrast with pausing patterns found in aphasia, providing a direct correlate in speech disfluency for a problem of configuring complete thoughts as encoded in clausal units. |
Exploitation Route | They can be used by anyone seeking to connect linguistic processing to psychiatric disease - i.e. psycholinguists, psychiatrists, psychologists, and philosophers - but also by doctors and therapists. |
Sectors | Healthcare |
Description | Scientific impact. This will be based on the papers we have now written up or are writing up, but is too early to assess. But even now, it is clear that this project is beginning to significantly impact on clinical diagnostic and therapeutic perspectives and practices, Impact on the public: The PI has been curator on a multi-million euro show in the science museum of Barcelona from March 2017 to February 2018, called Talking Brains, which had a dedicated subsection on language disintegration in major cognitive disorders and aphasia, thus opening a window into research on this project. The language-thought relationship, and the significance of language to mental health, is a central topic in this exhibition, which aims to raise awareness for this foundational question and its practical significance. In an associated conference series directed to the public, both the PI and project postdoc Dr Vitor Zimmerer participated, referencing the AHRC as a funder of our work. The exhibition attracted over 300.000 visitors and is planned to go on tour around the world. |
First Year Of Impact | 2017 |
Sector | Healthcare,Culture, Heritage, Museums and Collections |
Impact Types | Societal |
Title | database |
Description | Data is held on XL spreadsheets |
Type Of Material | Database/Collection of data |
Provided To Others? | No |
Impact | nil |
Description | Inferring information in schizophrenia: Evidence from a comic strip narration task |
Form Of Engagement Activity | A talk or presentation |
Part Of Official Scheme? | No |
Geographic Reach | National |
Primary Audience | Postgraduate students |
Results and Impact | Aims and hypotheses Patients with schizophrenia, including those with formal thought disorder, are thought to struggle with making inferences in various situations. Little has been done to assess this supposed deficit on goal oriented tasks. To assess how such patients comprehend inferred details and integrate them into a coherent narrative participants were asked to create a story from a cartoon. The hypotheses were that the participant groups would give different numbers of concrete narrative elements and inferred narrative elements when telling the story. Background There is evidence that patients with schizophrenia struggle to comprehend abstract cartoon jokes (which would require inference) rather than more concrete slapstick cartoon jokes. There is also evidence that patients with schizophrenia focus more on concrete details than inferred abstract details. Methods Thirty-three participants (9 control, 11 Schizophrenia, 5 thought disorder, 8 first degree relatives) were given an 8 slide wordless cartoon showing a couple preparing a dinner party but before the event the meal disappears so the host must find dinner elsewhere. Participants were told to tell the story and marked on the number of concrete and inferred details identified. Results There was a statistically significant difference (Kruskall Wallis test) between the groups in the number of inferred details reported (H(3)=11.855, p=.008). There was a significant difference (post-hoc Mann Whitney U test) between the control group (M=7.56) and thought disorder group (M=3.40) (U=15.133, p=.026), and also the first degree relatives (M=8.00) and the thought disorder groups (U=-16.925, p=.011). There was no significant difference between the groups in the number of concrete details reported (H(3)=3.634, p=.304). Conclusions Patients with schizophrenia do not seem to struggle with narrative inferences whereas those with formal thought disorder do. Patients with schizophrenia and first degree relatives performed as well as controls. There was no difference in ability to identify concrete details between the groups, although this may be due to an experimental error. These data provide basis for further study with a greater variety of cartoons. |
Year(s) Of Engagement Activity | 2016 |
Description | Invited submission to Parliamentary Inquiry into Cerebral Palsy |
Form Of Engagement Activity | Participation in an activity, workshop or similar |
Part Of Official Scheme? | Yes |
Geographic Reach | National |
Primary Audience | Policymakers/politicians |
Results and Impact | Sparked new ideas and new ways of thinking NA |
Year(s) Of Engagement Activity | 2014 |
Description | Language and thought |
Form Of Engagement Activity | A talk or presentation |
Part Of Official Scheme? | No |
Geographic Reach | International |
Primary Audience | Other audiences |
Results and Impact | This was an invitation to the Greater Philosophy Consortium, which is a conglomeration of philosophy departments with involvement from the general public. |
Year(s) Of Engagement Activity | 2016 |
Description | Magazine article |
Form Of Engagement Activity | A magazine, newsletter or online publication |
Part Of Official Scheme? | No |
Geographic Reach | International |
Primary Audience | Public/other audiences |
Results and Impact | Article for the Spanish popular magazine 'Mente y cerebro' (mind and brain) about importance of language in mental health research and our recent nature schizophrenia paper. |
Year(s) Of Engagement Activity | 2019 |
Description | Pauses in the speech profile of Schizophrenia |
Form Of Engagement Activity | A talk or presentation |
Part Of Official Scheme? | No |
Geographic Reach | National |
Primary Audience | Postgraduate students |
Results and Impact | AIM To examine the hypothesis that different sub-types of schizophrenia (SZ) and healthy controls, can be differentiated by the nature and frequency of pauses elicited during speech. BACKGROUND Disordered thought is a key symptom of SZ and is detected in a patient's speech. An important aspect of speech competence is pauses. Pauses during speech allow for discourse planning, word retrieval, dramatization and indicate breaks in the flow of speech and thought. Previous research has demonstrated that pauses occur in different syntactic locations, reflecting different discourse aims and cognitive processes, and their disruptions. METHOD 20 Participants: 5 SZ patients with thought disorder (SZ+TD), 5 SZ patients without thought disorder (SZ-TD), 5 first-degree-relatives (FDR's) of the SZ patients and 5 controls, performed a task in which they were required to describe a story portrayed in an eight slide cartoon strip. These descriptions were audio recorded and transcribed. Pauses were annotated by position in the sentence: 'utterance-initial', 'between-clause pauses', and 'within-clause' pauses. They were analysed non-parametrically in SPSS. RESULTS Statistical analysis revealed that SZ-TD participants used significantly more pauses than controls, p =0.032. SZ+TD and SZ-TD participants used significantly more pauses between utterances in comparison to controls, p=0.016 and p=0.032, respectively (calculated as a ratio of the total number of pauses). SZ-TD participants used significantly less within-clause pauses in comparison to controls, p = 0.08. In between-clause-pause condition, there were no significant differences between groups, p > .05. CONCLUSION The frequency and types of pauses differ significantly across SZ in comparison with healthy controls. Our data support the use of formal linguistic analysis and may have application in diagnostic and prognostic assessment in high-risk groups and potential development of talk-based therapies like CBT. In addition, the data support cognitive remediation adapted to have a focus on language and communication, with the prospect of improved day-to-day function in patients. |
Year(s) Of Engagement Activity | 2016 |
Description | Post-stroke speech disorders: foreign accent syndrome |
Form Of Engagement Activity | A press release, press conference or response to a media enquiry/interview |
Part Of Official Scheme? | No |
Geographic Reach | National |
Primary Audience | Media (as a channel to the public) |
Results and Impact | Sparked public interest NA |
Year(s) Of Engagement Activity | 2014 |
Description | Predicting Language |
Form Of Engagement Activity | A magazine, newsletter or online publication |
Part Of Official Scheme? | No |
Geographic Reach | International |
Primary Audience | Professional Practitioners |
Results and Impact | I wrote a brief article for the "Centre of Speech and Language Intervention Research" newsletter reporting our activities at London's Bloomsbury Festival. Aim of the newsletter is facilitation of links between research and clinical practice. |
Year(s) Of Engagement Activity | 2017 |
URL | https://www.ucl.ac.uk/speech-language-intervention-research/newsletters/CSLIR_Spring_2017_Newsletter |
Description | Predicting Language: an interactive installation part of London's Bloomsbury Festival |
Form Of Engagement Activity | Participation in an open day or visit at my research institution |
Part Of Official Scheme? | No |
Geographic Reach | Regional |
Primary Audience | Public/other audiences |
Results and Impact | When listening to language, we do not merely interpret what we heard. Instead, we "produce ahead", anticipating at grammatical and semantic levels what can come next. At the widely advertised Bloomsbury Festival 2016, with its focus on language, we offered visitors an opportunity to take part in a behavioural experiment which demonstrated this capacity. Our aims were to engage the public, share knowledge about language processing and make visitors think about how their language system works. Alongside the experiment the installation contained a short presentation about the language system. After the experiment, participants learned about their performance (so they could compare with friends) and received a badge as a souvenir. The installation was set up on the campus of the University College London on October 22nd 2016 from 11am to 5pm. About 150 people participated in the experiment, and we estimate that a total of 300 people visited the installation. The installation raised ,amy questions from the audience, mainly about the types of information processed when we produce and understand language, and about ways language can be disrupted due to different pathologies. Visitors reported that they found it exciting to engage with the experiment and the researchers. |
Year(s) Of Engagement Activity | 2016 |
URL | http://bloomsburyfestival.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2016/09/BF16-spreads-web-18-sept.pdf |
Description | Talk for doctors and parents |
Form Of Engagement Activity | A talk or presentation |
Part Of Official Scheme? | No |
Geographic Reach | International |
Primary Audience | Patients, carers and/or patient groups |
Results and Impact | Wolfram Hinzen : Talk at a Jornada Scientifica for doctors and parents organised by the Querer Foundation, Madrid (CaixaForum, 21 May 2018, "Primera Jornada neurocientífica y educativa enfocada a las enfermedades neurológicas y trastorno del lenguaje Infantil y juvenil"). |
Year(s) Of Engagement Activity | 2018 |
Description | Talking brains |
Form Of Engagement Activity | Participation in an activity, workshop or similar |
Part Of Official Scheme? | No |
Geographic Reach | International |
Primary Audience | Public/other audiences |
Results and Impact | This is a major exhibition project, arguably the first ever exhibition of language as a natural object or as an object of science. It is directly related to this grant as it centrally addresses language as a medium of thought and a means for understanding mental illness. Special installations are devoted to language schizophrenia, in autism, and in aphasia. The exhibition was bought at a multi-million level by the laCaixa foundation and will be on show for one year in Barcelona's CosmoCaixa's science museum, before it travels around the world. By now already, this exhibition has engaged mental health practicioners, speech therapists, patients, families of them, doctors, scientists, and students. |
Year(s) Of Engagement Activity | 2016,2017 |
Description | Use of ELAN to analyse video data for research and clinical practice. Speech & Language Therapy Special Interest Group |
Form Of Engagement Activity | A talk or presentation |
Part Of Official Scheme? | No |
Geographic Reach | Local |
Primary Audience | Professional Practitioners |
Results and Impact | Talk sparked questions and new understanding NA |
Year(s) Of Engagement Activity | 2014 |