Parental effects and the adaptive generation of helpers in a cooperative bird

Lead Research Organisation: UNIVERSITY OF EXETER
Department Name: Biosciences

Abstract

It is often said that one day, whether we like it or not, we will turn into one of our parents. Research in both laboratory animals and humans, suggests that this is not only because of our genetic similarities with our parents, but because our early life experiences shape the way we look and behave in adulthood. However, two major uncertainties exist in the development of these patterns. First, do we simply resemble our parents because we share the same environment? Second, do parents actually shape our development through varying their levels of investment in us, and if so, when does this happen and for whose benefit is it? To answer these questions we must use an animal in which we can ethically move offspring around in a natural setting at different stages of their development and then measure their behaviour in adulthood. I will use a 50g Australian bird that lives, like humans, in family groups in which offspring are reared by a variable number of helpers in addition to parents. I have been studying ~90 breeding groups of the chestnut-crowned babbler in the arid zone of south-eastern Australia for the last 8 years. This species is perfect for the aims of this project because mothers vary tremendously in their investment in offspring, varying the timing and amount of investment in eggs, as well as the number and sex of competitors with which offspring will be reared. Similarly, mothers, fathers and helpers vary significantly in how much food they provide to offspring after hatching, with some providing almost nothing and others contributing the majority of it. Finally, the success of a group depends on how many helpers it has: the survival of offspring increases until group size reaches 8, after which there are no further increases. Because helpers are almost always offspring from previous years, a small group of babblers should try to produce helpers, whereas a large group should try to produce dispersers that go off to start a new babbler group. By moving offspring between groups of different sizes and at different stages of their development, I will provide an answer to the following questions. (1) To what degree do mothers, fathers and helpers try to create helpful versus selfish offspring, and do they all agree when this should be? (2) Do offspring ignore this 'arrangement' or benefit from it, or are they being manipulated to behave in a way dictated by their up-bringing? (3) If one's upbringing influences the way they look and behave in adulthood, when is this effect generated, is it by the mum's before hatching, is it when they receive food in the nest, or is it determined after they leave the nest and begin to fend for themselves? The answers to these questions will shed new light on how the upbringing of animals and humans shapes the way we look and behave in adulthood and help answer the long-standing questions of what makes a good mother, father and helper and who is it good for? If, from these results, we are able to predict the cooperative versus selfish tendencies of offspring given their development, we can begin to direct future studies in humans to ask whether there is a sensitive period during child development that shapes our behaviour in adulthood? When is it and what types of experiences is it most sensitive to?

Planned Impact

For decades, developmental psychologists and evolutionary biologists have differed in answering the fundamental question of whether or not individual behaviour is governed by environment or genes. This is not a trivial question - with the advent of genetic engineering, the possibility of choosing genes for ones' own offspring and even the possible search for genes for criminality, it is now more important than ever to understand the role of early developmental conditions on future phenotype. Furthermore, there is renewed appreciation among evolutionary biologists of the profound role that the early environment can play in forming adulthood phenotype (morphology, physiology, behaviour) and influencing the rate of evolutionary change. One the most important early environments is that provided by the mother and other carers, for this is the first environment that a developing offspring experiences in the egg/womb and soon after.
Using the chestnut-crowned babbler, a 50g bird endemic to deserts of south-eastern Australia, I propose to conduct the first study investigating the role of maternal, paternal and helper investment in governing how cooperative or selfish offspring are in adulthood. Babblers, like most cooperative animals including humans, cooperate by protecting and providing food to offspring and defending their territory. Some individuals in groups contribute heavily and some do nothing at all- thus benefiting from the actions of others without paying a cost themselves. I propose that mothers (and other carers) influence the cooperative tendencies of offspring through differential allocation of resources. I expect the work outlined to benefit, both nationally and internationally:

(i) The academic community, by providing the first investigation of the role of parental influences on the evolution and maintenance of cooperation as well as individual levels of variation in their willingness to cooperate. The results of this study will be of interest to evolutionary ecologists since cooperation is an evolutionary enigma still in need of full explanation. The role of carer effects for the evolution of cooperation has not been investigated previously, and the full appreciation of the role of rearing effects for evolutionary processes is yet to be realised. The work will also be of interest to developmental psychologists who, for more than a century, have advocated the importance of early conditions for influencing individual personality in humans and animals.

(ii) Government regulatory bodies and economists, by elucidating whether upbringing helps reduce the tragedy of the commons. A tragedy of the commons arises when the selfish actions of individuals erode the cooperative or common good. The main challenges of the 21st century such as global warming, population growth and the inequitable use of resources are all tragedies of a common on a global scale, whereby each individual/community/nation fails to cooperate to avert potential disaster because of the fear that others will be selfish and fail to comply with negotiation. Understanding how animals solve the tragedy of the commons, will elucidate potential solutions to our current predicament.

(iii) The public who will benefit from understanding the importance of the rearing environment on offspring behaviour (and are as guilty as nations at committing a tragedy of the commons, albeit on a more local scale). The work will also be of general interest - the study of the evolution of behaviour is a tractable means to engage with the wider public.
This study will provide unique insights into the development of cooperative behaviour. We will provide greater understanding of the role of parents in influencing the cooperative tendencies of offspring, and as such elucidate how humans can begin to reduce the tragedy of the commons, which negatively affects the lives of all. As such this work is directly relevant to goals of the NERC, RCUK and WHO.

Publications

10 25 50

publication icon
Engesser S (2019) Chestnut-crowned babbler calls are composed of meaningless shared building blocks. in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

publication icon
Liebl AL (2016) Manipulating carer number versus brood size: complementary but not equivalent ways of quantifying carer effects on offspring. in Behavioral ecology : official journal of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology

publication icon
McAuliffe K (2015) When cooperation begets cooperation: the role of key individuals in galvanizing support. in Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences

 
Description The key findings to date are: (1) by changing mating patterns, mothers can procure extra help with rearing their offspring in a challenging ecology. In particular, females with no helpers, ie first time breeders, gain help either by chosing mates with helpers or failing this, mating multiply to attract help from multiple breeding males; (2) decisions to help are determined solely by kinship, rather than an attempt to show off parenting capabilities or as a form of rent payment for being allowed to remain on the natal territory beyond independence; and (3) our findings contradict a major emerging theory in the evolution of cooperative societies, that cooperation is selected among monogamously breeding parents. Not all of these results are yet published, but it is anticpated that they will be published in high impact journals and will have a signifciant impact in the field.
Exploitation Route The study aims at the first investigation of how mothers generate helpers in a cooperative vertebrate society. As such, the findings of this study are already inspiring other researchers to consider that altrusits in cooperative societies can be generated by parents by adaptively changing mating patterns and rearing conditions of offspring. It is anticpated that as the outcomes of the grant become published, the results will generate a major shift in the way that we view the evolution of cooperative societies, including in humans.
Sectors Education,Environment

 
Description The project aims at blue skies research and as such the primary beneficiaries are academics, post-graduate and under-graduate students. However, because the system under study is an arid zone specialist of outback Australia, it is anticipated that, in due course, the study will reveal how species occupying desert environments, which are explanding as a consequence of climate change, deal with their unique challanges. As the grant is little over a year old, the impact as yet is obviously limited.
First Year Of Impact 2014
Sector Education
Impact Types Societal